UDK 316.323.7:371.8(4-11) Reflections of the Word "Socialism" in Three Historical Periods and Three Countries VID PEČJAK1, SLOVENIA ANDRAS FARKAS, HUNGARY, JANA PUCHTOVA2, SLOVAKIA POVZETEK REFLEKSIJA NA BESEDO "SOCIALIZEM" V TREH ZGODOVINSKIH OBDOBJIH IN V TREH DEŽELAH V obdobju "realnega socializma" naj bi bila v vzhodnih evropskih deželah ideja socializma glavna vrednota takoimenovanega "novega človeka". Vprašanje pa je, koliko se je ta ideja v resnici vtisnila v zavest ljudi in koliko se je ohranila tudi po spremembi političnega sistema. Namen prve študije je bil odkriti in analizirati tipične besedne asociacije na besedo socializem pri ljubljanskih študentih v treh zgodovinskih obdobjih: leta 1969, 1991 in 1993. Namen druge študije pa je bil odkriti in analizirati proste besedne asociacije pri slovenskih, madžarskih in slovaških študentih v letu 1992. Beseda socializem je izzvala več afektivno in vrednostno pozitivnih odgovorov leta 1969 kot leta 1991 ter več leta 1993 kot leta 1991, istočasno pa manj negativnih asociacij leta 1969 kot 1991 ter manj leta 1993 kot 1991. Eden od možnih razlogov je razočaranje. Številna pričakovanja se niso uresničila, zato ljudje niso več tako zelo kritični do prejšnjega sistema. Pojavile so se tudi nekatere vsebinske spremembe. Kategorija ljudje je bila leta 1969 pomembnejša kot leta 1991 in 1993. Socializem je postal tudi bolj konkreten pojem, čeprav je njegova ideološka komponenta še vedno dominantna. Negativne asociacije so najbolj značilne za Slovake, nato Madžare in nato za Slovence. Eden od mogočih razlogov leži v razlikah njihovih nekdanjih političnih sistemov. V bivši Češkoslovaški je bil režim opresivnejši kot na Madžarskem ali v nekdanji Jugoslaviji. 1 The author collcctcd data for the first study, which is a part of a broader project 2 The authors, together with the first author, collected data for the second study, which is a part of a broader project Human values, attitudes, interests, opinions and similar subjective structures are constitutive parts of human spiritual culture. Moreover, there is no objective culture which is not transmitted and mediated by them. During the period of "real socialism" in East European countries the idea of socialism was prevalent. It was considered by official ideologists, politicians and educators as the prime value of the so-called new man. Any deviation from the official conception was criticized and often punished. From this standpoint all social phenomena were interpreted; even language, nations, religion and sexual relations. Yet the question is, how this official conception was reflected in one's mind, or, in another words, how much it penetrated internal values, attitudes and opinions? And how much is it preserved after the collapse of the regimes? The first study aims to find out and to compare the typical verbal associations given to the word socialism by Slovenian students in three important historical periods: in 1969, when the communist regime was firm and able to control most intellectual activity; in 1991, when the communist regime collapsed and some countries proclaimed their independence; and in 1993, when the new authority consolidated its state. The second study aims to find out and to compare the typical verbal associations given to the word socialism by Slovenian, Hungarian and Slovakian students in 1992. The method of free association was chosen because of some advantages over the method of interview or questionnaires. They investigate only the surface structure of attitudes, values and opinions, whereas the method of free association discloses also their deep structure. When subjects answer the questions, like "do you agree with the idea of socialism: yes, moderately, not at all," they are much more conscious about their response. But when they react with "the first word, which appears in their mind after hearing the word socialism," they do not know or know only vaguely that they are expressing their attitudes, values and other structures which mediate the responses. Therefore the self-controlling mechanism is weaker. There are also some other advantages of the indirect techniques such as the method of association. The subject is free to answer with any word which appears in his or her mind, whereas in the interview these possibilities are limited. The method of association discloses not only the evaluative component of the internal structures, but also the denotative one. It detects its semantic content more exhaustively than any other technique. Method First study: In the framework of a broader investigation (Szalay and Pečjak, 1979), the data were collected from student groups at the University of Ljubljana and University of Maryland in 1969. In Ljubljana the testing was repeated in 1991 and in 1993. Each group consisted of 50 students, males and females. Second study: In 1992 the association test was performed in four national student groups: Slovenian, Slovak, Hungarian and Spanish. The testing was a part of some broad cross-cultural investigation. The Spanish group was included later for comparison because of some similarities to the three East European groups (Spain too had a totalitarian role and the level of development is similar). The associative Group Analysis Technique, developed by L. Szalay and J. Brent (1967), was used. The subjects were asked to give as many responses as they could in one minute to the word socialism (besides some other words, e.g. capitalism, which are not treated in the paper). The subjects were told to avoid responding to their own previous associations. Most subjects gave 7 to 10 responses. Here is one example: Stimulus word: SOCIALISM Individual responses: Communism C4Lit liU utajUa, fuut TXoax By excluding from the further analyses all the responses, given by only one member of the group, the AGA technique deals only with more stable, shared responses, giving more reliable information about trends of associations. Each response is given a score indicating weighted order of its occurence. The scale of scoring was elaborated by Szalay on the basis of stability of the rank of responses. The weights assigning to responses beginning with the first in the sequence are: 6, 5, 4, 3, 3, 3, 3, 2, 2, 1, 1. Group scores (frequencies and the sums of weights for particular responses) indicate the salience of responses for the individual groups of subjects. Here is one example: SOCIALISM Some group data for the Slovenes f sw Yugoslavia 16 82 communism 18 72 Tito 16 52 Stalin 11 50 Utopia 7 23 decline 7 22 red 7 22 The responses are grouped into categories based on their semantic similarities; e.g. the responses "Yugoslavia" and "Russia" are grouped together into the category entitled "countries", and the responses "bad" and "cruel" into the category "negative references". Results The scores of each category and each concept were established and compared across the Slovenian groups in three periods and A cross four national groups. About one third of differences is significant (< .05). First study: the scores of the Slovenian groups in three historical periods: 1993 1991 1969 USA 19693 People, society 103 110 214 Ideas, Symbols 94 127 95 Social systems 109 90 129 (83) Negative references 137 165 47 (19) Positive references 74 56 126 Countries 142 75 101 (169) Names 98 40 81 (88) Dominant values (the responses with the highest scores): 1993: Yugoslavia 72, communism 62, Tito 58, poverty 30, past 28 1991: Capitalism 55, Yugoslavia 45, Utopia 38, decay 33, east 30 1969: Yugoslavia 76, capitalism 63, communism 61, Engels 57, Tito 46 Second study: the scores of the national groups Slovenia Slovakia Hungary Spain Negative references 134 251 113 63 Political references 204 122 104 116 Countries 198 31 43 0 Names 160 23 121 37 Ideas 76 63 97 18 Positive references 64 38 95 151 Economic references 18 33 34 37 Time 27 19 54 0 Authority, power 14 20 38 0 People 23 32 35 35 Dominant values: Slovenia: Yugoslavia 84, communism 72, Tito 52, Stalin 50, east 43, Slovakia: totalitarian 41, restriction 32, communism 28, Utopia 24, USSR 20. Hungary: Utopia 51, equality 47, Kadar 30, Soviet Union 29, communism, 21 (ideology 21). Spain: equality 101, left 34, democracy 36, freedom 35, Marx 23. 3 The U.S. data arc taken only partialJy into consideration because only some categories are the same and direcUy comparable with the categories of the Slovenians. Index or Evaluative Dominance. BED = 2* % re»P. 2,f all resp. Slovenia 1969: + .46 Slovenia 1991: - .49 Slovenia 1993: - .29 Slovenia: - .36 Slovakia: - .74 Hungary: - .09 Spain: + .41 The Index of Evaluative dominance is considered by Szalay as an index of the evaluative component of attitudes and corresponds to the evaluative factor of the Semantic Differential. It varies from -1 to +1. Discussion Slovenians in 1969,1991,1993: The word socialism elicited more positive associations in 1969 than in 1991, and in 1993 more than in 1991, though still less than in 1969. At the same time it elicited less negative associations in 1969 than in 1991, and in 1993 less than in 1991, though not so little as in 1969. One possible reason could be disappointment. People's expectancies have not been fulfilled and many strange things have come to pass (e.g. unemployment and corruption). Therefore they are not so critical of the former socialist system as they were in 1991. The results do not agree with the findings of M. Ule and Vlado Miheljak (1993), who found a pretty negative attitude towards socialism. They applied the interview method among 17 years old high school pupils. The difference can be due to different characteristics of groups (university students are a selected group) or to different methods of detection. It is possible that on the surface they are conforming to the prevalent social trend, but internally their refusal of socialist idea is less pronounced. In 1969 the category people was more salient than in 1991 and in 1993, when the responses like worker and working class almost disappeared. In 1993 socialism became a more concrete concept (high scores for countries and names), but the ideological component (ideas, symbols) has remained strong too. The national groups: The negative score of IED is the highest for Slovakia, then Slovenia and then Hungary. One possible reason are the differences of their former socialism. In Czechoslovakia the regime was much more repressive than in Hungary with its "Goulash Socialism" or in Yugoslavia with its "Selfmanaging Socialism". It is harder to explain the difference between Hungarian and Slovenian data. It could reflect the political situation in these countries. In both of them the change of system was relatively gradual and peaceable, yet in Hungary it happened earlier and it was initiated by some communists who rehabilitated the revolution in 1956. The Spanish IED is the only one which is positive. Spaniards do not have experiences with "real socialism"; they have more experiences with faschism, which is considered to be on the opposite side of socialism. Besides, they probably connect socialism more with their own socialist party, though the name Marx is frequently mentioned. For the Slovenian group the word socialism has quite concrete denotation, and it is more saturated with political meaning than for the others (especially for Spaniards). Its meaning is also slightly less saturated with economic component. These reactions could reflect the strong influence of politics in today's Slovenia. For all three East European groups, and especially for the Hungarian group, socialism is still a very ideological concept. Utopia is a common association. This denotation is probaly the result of past indoctrination. Literature Szalay L.B., Brent J.E., The analysis of cultural meanings through free associations. Journal of Social Psychology,72,161-187,1967. Szalay L.B., Peijak V., A comparative analysis of US and Slovenian sociopolitiai frame of rcferencc. Political Anthropology, Mouton, 354-389,1979. Ule M., Miheijalc V., Mladina 1993, Republika 1993