Psihološka obzorja / Horizons of Psychology, 23, 136-144 (2014) CC = 3550 © Društvo psihologov Slovenije, ISSN 2350-5141 UDK = 37.015 Znanstveni empiričnoraziskovalni prispevek / Scientific empirical article The IPOO-model of creative learning and the students' information processing characteristics Katalin Mezo* and Ferenc Mezo University of Debrecen, Hajdûbôszôrmény, Hungary Abstract: The present study was designed to examine secondary school students' information processing characteristics during learning and their relationship with the students' academic averages, internal motivation for learning and cognitive abilities, such as intelligence and creativity. Although many studies have previously focused on this issue, we are now studying this topic from the perspective of the IPOO-model, which is a new theoretical approach to school learning (note: IPOO is an acronym of Input, Process, Output, Organizing). This study featured 815 participants (secondary school students) who completed the following tests and questionnaires: Raven's Advanced Progressive Matrices (APM) intelligence test, the 'Unusual Uses' creativity test (UUT), the 2nd version of the Jupiterbolha-proba (Jupiter Flea test - JB2) to test the information processing method of learning, and the Learning Attitude Questionnaire (LAQ). In our analysis we took the gender, school grade and academic average of participants into account. According to our results, the quality of information-processing methods of learning is at a low level, and there are no significant strong correlational relationships among the tests and questionnaire results (except in the cases of fluency, originality, and flexibility). There were no significant differences between genders or classes. These findings are consistent with the findings of previous studies. Keywords: information processing, creativity, learning, IPOO-model, secondary school students IPOO-model ustvarjalnega učenja in značilnosti procesiranja informacij pri učencih Katalin Mezo in Ferenc Mezo University of Debrecen, Hajduboszormeny, Madžarska Povzetek: Namen raziskave je bil preveriti, kako učenci v srednji šoli procesirajo informacije med učenjem in kakšen je odnos med značilnostmi procesiranja in njihovo učno uspešnostjo, notranjo motivacijo in kognitivnimi sposobnostmi, kot sta inteligentnost in ustvarjalnost. Čeprav so bile na tem področju v preteklosti že opravljene raziskave, se pričujoča študija te problematike loteva z vidika IPOO-modela, ki predstavlja nov teoretski pristop k šolskemu učenju (IPOO je akronim za Input (vnos), Process (proces), Output (izdelek), Organizing (organizacija)). V raziskavi je sodelovalo 815 dijakov, ki so izpolnili naslednje teste oziroma vprašalnike: Ravenove zahtevne matrice (APM), Test ustvarjalnosti »Nenavadne uporabe« (UUT), Jupiterbolha-proba test - druga verzija (JB2) za testiranje učne metode procesiranja informacij in Vprašalnik učnih stališč (LAQ). V raziskavo smo kot spremenljivke vključili tudi spol, razred in povprečno učno uspešnost sodelujočih učencev. Rezultati so pokazali nizek nivo kakovosti učne metode procesiranja informacij in nepovezanost med testnimi rezultati ter rezultati na vprašalnikih (razen v primeru fluentnosti, originalnosti in fleksibilnosti). Razlike med spoloma in razredi se niso izkazale kot statistično značilne. Ugotovitve so skladne z ugotovitvami predhodnih študij. Ključne besede: procesiranje informacij, ustvarjalnost, učenje, IPOO model, dijaki *Naslov/Address: Katalin Mezö, Department of Psychology and Special Education, Faculty of Child and Adult Education, University of Debrecen, Hajdüböszörmeny, Hungary, e-mail: kata.mezo1@gmail.com In the studies and theories which focus on the information processing related to creativity (for example early works of Cropley, 1997; Davis, 1992; Helmholtz, 1986; Kris, 1953; Stein, 1974; Wallas, 1926; Whiting, 1958; and others), we can find descriptions of a kind of neutral, general creative thinking which exists without specifically defined creative areas (e.g. cooking, painting, writing etc.). These theories and descriptions are generally sufficient, and are applicable in numerous situations; however, we must recognise that more specific models of creative process may be needed in different areas of culture and/or talent. One such area is school learning. However, creative learning is distinct from general creativity (which can be characterized by fluency, originality and flexibility), because of its focus on the creative information processing procedure involved in learning itself (Craft, Cremin, Burnard, & Chappell, 2007; Jeffrey, 2006). On the other hand, there are different models of school learning (for example Bloom, 1968; Bruner, 1961; Carroll, 1963; Galperin, 1989), but these models do not concentrate sufficiently on creative learning. In addition, neutral, general creativity is a frequent, but not central variable in studies of school learning (Cropley, 2003). In these studies general creativity is only one variable of the other background factors (for example intelligence, motivation, gender, age or school grades) of learning. In this article we study learning from the perspective of the IPOO-model, which is a new theoretical approach to creative school learning (Mezo, 2011). The IPOO-model of Learning According to the IPOO-model (built on a cognitive psychology basis), school learning is an information processing procedure, and has four components: input, process, output and organizing (Mezo, 2011). Every component is built on the basis of special abilities, motivations, and methods. Learning problems can be found in every component of the IPOO-model. For example some typical learning problems of the input phase are "What will the topic of learning be?", or "Where can I find information about my learning topic?", or "How can I use the authorities (authoritative sources) effectively?'" Learning problems can occur in the process phase of learning - e.g. "I don't understand the text, or the lesson!" or "I have to learn too much. I cannot memorize everything..." Subsequently, learning problems can be encountered in the output phase, for example, "I hate oral exams.", "How can I write my dissertation?", or "How can I apply my knowledge in my life?" Last, but not least, organising-problems can include the classical problems of the time, the place and the cost of learning etc. In this learning model, we can differentiate three information-processing possibilities based on the relationship between input and output: the information-processing method of learning may be reproductive, productive, or cause information loss (Table 1). The main indication of deficient learning is that quantitative and qualitative characteristics feature less in the output (e.g. in an oral exam) than in the input (e.g. in a book), i.e. input > output. If the input is greater than the output, the learning will be ineffective. For example, a poem has four stanzas (as input), but the learner can reproduce (as output) less than four verses. The most important characteristic of learning of this kind is information deficit. In the case of reproductive learning, the input is same as the output (input = output). If the input is equal to the output, the learning will be reproductive without any creative information processing. For example, a poem has four stanzas (as input), and the learner can recite (as output) all of the four stanzas, but she/he does not understand the words, the stanzas, the poem, or the metaphors within the poem, etc. The learner tries to memorise the lesson word by word without the need for understanding, or basic processing. Finally, productive learning can be described by the following inequality: input < output. If the input is less than the output, learning will be meaningful, holistic and creative. For example, a poem has four stanzas (as input), and the learner (at the moments of output) can recite all of the four stanzas and she/he understands the poem, and she/he searches for the nexus between the new lesson and his/her earlier knowledge. In the case of the IPOO-model, the learning ideal is creative, productive learning. If students' learning styles cause information loss or are reproductive, then learning development is needed (e.g. our applied learning developmental program is the "IPOO-minimum program"). Background Factors of Learning The intrapersonal factors of learning have been studied for several decades. The most frequently identified factors of these studies include intelligence, creativity, and motivation. Researchers often use these factors to predict the learning achievement which is generally operationalized as an academic average. We provide a brief overview of these factors in the following section. The role of intelligence in learning achievement has been a central question since the dawn of intelligence testing (consider, for example, the creation of Binet's intelligence test). According to a number of studies (e.g. Neisser et al., 1996; Sternberg & Grigorenko, 2007), there is a relationship between IQ scores and school achievement (the value of the correlation is about r = .50). Researchers have long disagreed about the existence of gender differences in overall intelligence (Petersen & Hyde, 2010). While we can find studies which have reported gender differences (e.g. Furnham, Clark , & Bailey, 1999; Hyde, 2005; Lynn, Irwing & Cammock, 2002), other studies find no gender differences in intelligence (e.g. Brody, 1992; Halpern & LaMay, 2000; Jensen, 1998). Researchers have been studying the relationship between creativity and learning since the fifties of Table 1. Examples of different processing approaches to an input (e.g. a school subject) text, and their possible outputs (the interpretative frame is the IPOO-model of learning) INPUT: Let us imagine that the text of a school subject... PROCESS by: OUTPUT: In the case of an oral/written exam, we can observe that the learner communicates... Productive (creative): re-editing ... a creative product: a non-confused text ...is a confused text (from the perspective of a learner or anybody else) Reproductive: mechanical memorising of the input without re-editing (but re-editing is possible in the future) .a confused text (identical to the input text) Information loss: not enough memorisation of the input text, making it impossible to re-edit ...nothing, or a more confused text than the it in the future (without re-learning, of course) original input text ...is a mix of essential and non-essential explicit information Productive (creative): highlighting essential information and rejecting non-essential information .a creative product: highlighting the essence Reproductive: mechanical memorisation of the input (without highlighting essentials - but highlighting is possible in the future) .a text identical to the input Information loss: not enough memorisation of essential and non-essential information of the input text so it is impossible to recall this in the future ... a text with less explicit information than the input ...contains implicit information Productive (creative): creating explicit conclusions ...creative products: explicit conclusions based on the text Reproductive: mechanical memorisation of the input (without creating explicit conclusions - but this is possible in the future) ... a text identical to the input (without explicit conclusions) Information loss: not enough memorisation of the input text, making it impossible to find explicit conclusions in the future ... a text that contains less implicit information than the input ...contains atomistic information in different paragraphs and/or chapters Productive (creative): finding relationships among different paragraphs, texts and prior learning ... a creative product: holistic knowledge Reproductive: mechanical memorising of the input (without looking for relations among pieces of information - but this is possible in the future) ... a text that is a set of atomistic pieces of information Information loss: not enough memorisation of the atomistic information of the input text so it is impossible to find relationships among non-memorised pieces of information in the future ...a text that is an incomplete set of atomistic pieces of information from the input text the 20th century, when Joy Paul Guilford proposed the "Structure of Intelligence" model of human intelligence (Guilford, 1950, 1962). According to Guilford, creativity is important from a learning perspective, but it is kept in the background during school learning. Later studies have not been able to refute this (Feist & Runco, 1993; Lucas, 2001; Runco, 2007; Sternberg & Lubart, 1999). The correlation between creativity and academic achievement is usually r < .30 (Cropley, 2003). Baer (2012) reviewed more than 80 studies related to gender differences in creativity and concluded that the results of these studies are quite contradictory and the evidence does not clearly support gender differences. In terms of learning motivation, according to Józsa (2000), the relationship between school achievement and learning motivation changes during the school years, with the two are becoming more and more independent of each other as the years pass. According to Gordon and Austin (2002), "...the examinations of gender and achievement differences in motivation have histories that span at least three decades" (p. 293). As in the case of most psychological variables with a history stretching back several decades, the results are sometimes contradictory in the area of learning motivation, too. One of the few areas of consensus on this topic is that, in an ideal case, students have internal motivations for learning (Ryan & Deci, 2000; Sullo, 2007). As we have seen, research of the intrapersonal background factors of learning has a tradition stretching back several decades. A number of researchers have focused on the predictive roles and correlations of cognitive abilities (e.g. intelligence and creativity) and non-cognitive factors (e.g. motivation) from the perspective of school achievement (e.g. academic average). Although these are classical and traditional variables of learning research, from time to time it is necessary to study these variables from the perspective of new learning theories (for example the IPOO-model of learning). Therefore, the aim of our study was to gain insight into secondary school students' characteristics in terms of information processing during (creative, productive) learning, and to analyse the relationships between these characteristics and cognitive abilities, internal motivation, and academic average. According to our classroom observations, these factors are fairly independent from each other, so we cannot use them as predictors of each other. We also addressed the issue of gender equality. Method Participants Participants were 9th to 12th grade students with a mean age of 16.01 years (SD = .97), attending two middle secondary schools in Hungary. The total number of participants was 815. The sample was gender heterogeneous; about 25% of students (n = 202) were male. Two hundred and thirty-eight students were 9th graders (M = 14.96; SD = .42), 261 were 10th graders (M = v age 7 o \ age 16.08; SD = .65), 218 students were 11th graders (Mage = 16.91; SD = .38), and 98 students were 12th graders (M^ = 17.60; SD = .52). Although this is an ad hoc sample, which is not representative enough, we think that this study can provide sufficient preliminary results for further examinations. Instruments Creative learning was measured with the Hungarian version of the JB2 test (the Jupiterbolha-proba or, in English, the "Jupiter Flea" test; Mezo, 2011). Its subscales refer to re-editing ("Do students create a new and systematically logical structure when they encounter a confused text?"), direct info-management ("Do students find the direct essential information in a text?"), indirect info-management ("Do students create conclusions?"), and holistic learning ("Do students look for relationships with their knowledge?"). The total score is the average of these subscales. For the purpose of this study, we used mainly the "total score" variable of this test to measure creative learning. The reliability of the total score of the JB2 was a = .78 - .87 (Mezo, 2011). We used Raven's Advanced Progressive Matrices test (APM; Raven, Raven & Court, 1998) for measuring general intelligence - for more information about the Hungarian adaptation of Raven's APM, see Rozsa (2006) and Mezo and Kurucz (2014). Some researchers believe that scores on the Raven's Progressive Matrices test give the best estimate of general intelligence (the "g factor"; see Jensen, 1998; McLaurin, Jenkins, Farrar & Rumore, 1973). The APM has two different parts: APM-I (with 12 items) and APM-II (containing an extra 36 items for those who are good at APM-I). The total APM score is the sum of these parts. APM reliability is a = .85. Fluency, originality and flexibility (as classical creativity variables) were tested by applying the Unusual Uses Test (a subtest of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking, TTCT; Torrance, 1966), based on Guilford's Alternative Uses Task (Guilford, 1962). In this task subjects are asked to list as many uses as possible for a common household item (such as a brick, a pencil or a key). This task is the "Unusual Uses" subtest of the TTCT and it is used to measure such components of creative thinking as fluency (the sum of relevant answers), originality (the infrequency of responses), flexibility (differences in the categories of the responses), average originality (originality/fluency), and relative flexibility (flexibility/fluency). Barkoczi and Zetenyi (1981) provide information about the adaptation of the "Unusual Uses" test for the Hungarian population. The a value of these tests ranges between .70 and .90 (Cropley, 2003). Internal learning motivation was assessed by applying the Learning Attitude Questionnaire (LAQ; Kontra, 2006) which consists of 31 items with a five-point response scale (1 = it is non typical of me; 5 = it is typical of me). There are six subscales: further education ambitions, interest, predisposition to industrious learning, learning for a good rating, learning for a good position among peers, and learning for rewards. We used an "internal learning motivation" variable based on the mean of the first three subscales (the Cronbach a is .81; Kontra, 2006). Our control variables were gender (male/female) and grades (grade 9th, 10th, 11th, and 12th). Procedure Participation was not obligatory in this study. The students and their parents were informed about the subject of the study before testing and they were able to make a decision about whether to participate or not. Consequently, our subjects had some kind of internal motivation to participate in this examination, so we did not need to use external motivators (e.g. money, good marks etc.) in order to involve participants. We met our participants on four different occasions during this examination. In the first session, we tested creative thinking by applying the Unusual Uses Test and Table 2. Descriptive statistics and tests of normality of the examined variables Scalesa Min Max Mdn M SD Skewnessb Kolmogorov-Kurtosisb Smirnov Z Academic average 2.10 5.00 3.90 3.86 0.57 -0.24 -0.52 1.54* JB2: Re-editing 0.00 100.00 25.00 29.74 24.34 0.66 -0.22 4.62* JB2: Direct info-management 0.00 100.00 62.50 56.58 20.82 -0.34 -0.44 4.29* JB2: Indirect info-management 0.00 100.00 50.00 45.57 21.74 0.09 -0.32 3.27* JB2: Holistic learning 0.00 100.00 25.00 30.77 21.03 0.21 -0.73 3.56* JB2: Total Score 3.13 93.75 40.62 40.66 16.16 0.22 -0.29 1.75* APM: Total Score 11.00 47.00 31.00 30.36 5.39 -0.59 0.45 2.59* LAQ: Internal motivation 1.80 4.60 3.40 3.37 0.49 -0.27 0.08 1.90* UUT: Fluency 1.00 48.00 9.00 10.08 5.29 1.14 3.58 2.64* UUT: Originality 0.37 29.00 5.27 5.80 3.49 1.54 4.82 2.36* UUT: Flexibility 0.00 19.00 8.00 7.81 3.53 0.40 -0.08 2.23* UUT: Average originality 0.17 3.00 0.53 0.59 0.26 3.60 19.43 6.44* UUT: Relative flexibility 0.00 12.00 0.83 0.83 0.43 20.95 535.67 9.51* a JB2: Jupiter Flea test, APM: Advanced Progressive Matrices test, LAQ: Learning Attitude Questionnaire, UUT: Unusual Uses test b SESk = .09; SEK t . = .17 Skewness 5 Kurtosis *p < .05 we also applied the Learning Attitude Questionnaire. On the second occasion participants filled in the APM test. The JB2 was applied during the third session. At the final session, we discussed the results with the participants. Every session lasted around 45 minutes, and was held at their school. Results According to the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test (Table 2), the distributions of researched variables were not normal (p < .05 in all cases). However, the values of skewness and kurtosis are near zero in several cases (the absolute value of the difference from zero is less than 1 for skewness and this difference is less than 3 for kurtosis), so we could use parametric tests for these variables. However, the skewness and kurtosis of about half of our variables are higher, therefore we consistently used non-parametric statistical tests. Table 2 shows that the mean values of JB2 variables and information-processing achievements are at fairly low levels. The results of about 75% of the participants are less than 50% in re-editing and holistic learning. Approximately 50% of participants scored less than 50% in the case of indirect info-management. It seems that direct info-management was the easiest task of the JB2 because a significant proportion of participants achieved a result above 50%. Overall, the JB2 total score was less than 50% in most cases. Table 3. Mean ranks and the results of the Kruskal Wallis test for testing the differences on the used variables between the school grades Variables Mean rank of grade 9th 10th 11th 12th X2 (df = 3) Academic average 401.79 376.49 441.19 433.17 10.33* Re-editing 372.30 381.68 452.28 466.29 23.10* Direct info-management 355.37 392.79 441.18 502.53 34.21* Indirect info-management 382.58 393.69 417.88 485.84 15.28* Holistic learning 352.07 432.98 422.64 444.74 20.19* Total Score 348.25 396.87 444.39 501.80 36.80* APM 362.18 428.18 433.58 408.64 13.56* Internal motivation 417.25 407.54 402.71 398.51 0.64 Fluency 400.03 374.35 442.27 440.74 12.17* Originality 411.89 413.88 402.19 395.81 0.62 Flexibility 406.26 363.30 442.75 453.96 18.04* Average originality 426.92 458.73 360.06 328.94 33.79* Relative flexibility 419.09 392.00 410.69 417.69 1.97 *p < .05 The IPOO-model of creative learning Table 4. Spearman's rho correlations between all examined variables Variables^